Corrections and clarifications

There is an error in Figure 3.3. This is the tree diagram representing Eddy's (1982) medical problem. For the branch representing P(Negative | Cancer) the probability should be 0.208, NOT 0.028. Accordingly, the number at the rightmost end of the branch should be 0.0028, NOT 0.00028.


This does not affect the answer to the problem stated in the text, which remains 0.077.


My thanks to Chloe Turner (one of my students) for pointing this out.

Sunday 7 June 2009

Perceptions of randomness: Not so irrational?

A Psychological Review paper by Ulrike Hahn and Paul Warren suggests that people's perceptions of randomness are not as irrational as is frequently supposed. Specifically, they argue that these perceptions are what would be expected from organisms with limited short-term memory capacity encountering fairly short sequences of random events.

To illustrate their argument they analyse the likelihood of encountering particular sequences of coin tosses. With a sequence of four coin tosses, there are 16 possible sequences that could occur (e.g. HHHH, HHHT, THHT,... etc.). Suppose, though, that we are interested in the occurrences of two equally-likely subsequences, HHH and HHT, within all the possible four-toss outcomes. We can analyse this by representing all 16 sequences in a probability tree diagram, such that HHHH is one branch, HHHT another branch, and so on.

Notice that the subsequence HHH occurs twice within one branch (HHHH), whereas this never happens for HHT. This result generalizes to much longer sequences of coin tosses. Hahn and Warren describe this as being like waiting for a bus: "For a long and frustrating period, there is no bus in sight, and then, all of a sudden, several arrive in immediate succession" (p.455). The upshot of this analysis is that you would need to wait longer (on average) to encounter HHH in a sequence of coin tosses than you would HHT. In a sequence of coin tosses, the expected wait time for HHH is 14 coin tosses, whereas for HHT it is just 8.

The authors also argue that this theoretical analysis maps quite well onto human experience. Limited lifespan and resources mean that random events we encounter are likely to be relatively short sequences, as compared to the long runs discussed in explanations of probability. Furthermore, short-term memory has a very limited capacity, so people can only hold a few events in mind. Hahn and Warren extend their analysis by reporting the results of a computer simulation designed to assess the probability that certain substrings will not occur within a given sequence of coin tosses. They specifically looked at the likelihood of the following substrings: HHHH, HTHH, HHHT, HHTT, and HTHT.

The substring with the highest probability of non-occurrence was HHHH, and the next substring that was likely to not occur was HTHT. Likewise, when Hahn and Warren did the same analysis for substrings of length 6, the most likely substrings to not occur were HHHHHH and HTHTHT. Significantly, previous research has shown that naive participants tend to regard these sequences as less likely to have been generated by a random process than more varied or less regular strings.

In short, Hahn and Warren argue that people's misperceptions of chance are a rational response to the environments that they encounter (although they are still errors). The gambler's fallacy can be viewed in this light also. This fallacy occurs if (say) a gambler believes that a sequence of HHH means that T is more likely to occur on the next toss of the coin. However, Hahn and Warren's analysis shows that a gambler is likely to encounter the sequence HHHT before he or she encounters the sequence HHHH, even though if the gambler has just experienced HHH then the next outcome is equally likely to be H or T.

Reference

Hahn, U., and Warren, P.A. (2009). Perceptions of randomness: Why three heads are better than four. Psychological Review, 116 (2), 454-461.

No comments:

About Google Analytics

This website uses Google Analytics, a web analytics service provided by Google, Inc. (“Google”). Google Analytics uses “cookies”, which are text files placed on your computer, to help the website analyze how users use the site. The information generated by the cookie about your use of the website (including your IP address) will be transmitted to and stored by Google on servers in the United States . Google will use this information for the purpose of evaluating your use of the website, compiling reports on website activity for website operators and providing other services relating to website activity and internet usage. Google may also transfer this information to third parties where required to do so by law, or where such third parties process the information on Google's behalf. Google will not associate your IP address with any other data held by Google. You may refuse the use of cookies by selecting the appropriate settings on your browser, however please note that if you do this you may not be able to use the full functionality of this website. By using this website, you consent to the processing of data about you by Google in the manner and for the purposes set out above.